- Obtener vínculo
- X
- Correo electrónico
- Otras apps
- Obtener vínculo
- X
- Correo electrónico
- Otras apps
The chances of a commercial
airliner crashing are vanishingly small -- and yet many people are
uncomfortable flying. Vaccination for many common childhood diseases entail
almost no risk -- but parents still worry. Human perception of probabilities --
especially very small and very large probabilities -- can be markedly distorted
and these distortions can lead to potentially disastrous decisions.
But why we distort probability is
unclear. While the question has been previously studied, there is no consensus
on its causes.
A team of scientists from New
York University and Peking University, using experimental research, has now
concluded that our cognitive limitations lead to probability distortions and to
subsequent errors in decision-making. The researchers have developed a model of
human cognitive limitations and tested its predictions experimentally, as
reported in the latest issue of the journal Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences.
The team, which included New York
University’s Laurence Maloney as well as the University of Peking University’s
Hang Zhang, a professor, and Xiangjuan Ren, a post-doctoral fellow, initiated
the analysis by examining the nature of distortions as a potential clue for
explaining this phenomenon.
“Probability distortion limits
human performance in many tasks, and we conjectured that the observed changes
in probability distortion with task was a kind of partial compensation for
human limitations,” explains Maloney. “A marathon runner with a sprained ankle
will not run as well as she might have with ankle intact, but the awkward,
limping gait we observe could in fact be an optimal compensation for injury.”
The key step in the model is the
recoding of probabilities that depends on the range of probabilities in a
task.
“Much like a variable
magnification microscope, the brain can represent a wide range of
probabilities, but not very accurately, or a narrow range at high precision,”
explains Maloney. “If, for example, a task involves reasoning about the
probability of various causes of death, for example, then the probabilities are
all very small (thankfully) and small differences are important. We can set the
microscope to give us high resolution over a limited window of very small
probabilities. In another task we might accept less precision in return for the
ability to represent a much wider range of probabilities.”
Zhang, Ren, and Maloney set out
to test this model in two experiments, one in which subjects made typical
economic decisions under risk (e.g. choosing between a 50:50 chance of $200 and
the certainty of $70) and one involving judgements of relative frequency (the
relative frequency of black and white dots appearing on a computer screen). The
two experiments together tapped into the basic ways we use probability and
frequency in everyday life. The researchers found that their model predicted
human performance far better than any previous model.
Comentarios
Publicar un comentario